Gender Differences in Moral Orientation
Psychological Bulletin 2000, Vol. 126, No. 5, 703-726
Copyright 2000 by the American Psychological Association, Inc. 0033-2909/00*5.00 DOI; 10.1037/TO33-2909.126.5.703
Gender Differences in Moral Orientation: A Meta-Analysis
Sara Jaffee and Janet Shibley Hyde University of Wisconsin—Madison
C. Gilligan’s (1982) critique of L. Kohlberg’s theory of moral reasoning and her assertion that two modes
of moral reasoning (justice and care) exist have been the subject of debate within the field of psychology
for more than 15 years. This meta-analysis was conducted to review quantitatively the work on gender
differences in moral orientation. The meta-analysis revealed small differences in the care orientation
favoring females (d = -.28) and small differences in the justice orientation favoring males (d = .19).
Together, the moderator variables accounted for 16% of the variance in the effect sizes for care reasoning
and 17% of the variance in the effect sizes for justice reasoning. These findings do not offer strong
support for the claim that the care orientation is used predominantly by women and that the justice
orientation is used predominantly by men.
The 1982 publication of Carol Gilligan’s In a Different Voice
marked one of those rare moments when social science research
breaches the ivied walls of academia and captures the public’s
imagination. Gilh’gan’s assertion that females and males speak in
different moral voices—a care voice characterized by the desire to
maintain relationships and to respond to others’ needs and a justice
voice characterized by considerations of fairness and equity—
resonated with readers’ experience (Mednick, 1989). Ms. maga-
zine named Gilligan its 1984 Woman of the Year, lauding her for
research that “created a new appreciation for a previously uncata-
logued female sensibility, as well as possibilities for new under-
standing between the genders” (Van Gelder, 1984, p. 37). In 1996,
Time magazine included Gilligan among its “Time 25″—25 inno-
vative Americans with the ability “to show us the world anew, to
educate and entertain us, to change the way we think about
ourselves and others” (‘Time 25,” 1996, p. 54). Wrote the editors,
How likely is it that a single book could change the rules of psychol-
ogy, change the assumptions of medical research, change the conver-
sation among parents and teachers and developmental professionals
about the distinctions between men and women, boys and girls? (p. 66)
More than 15 years after the publication of this influential work,
are its theses supported by available scientific data?
Gilligan’s (1982) research on female moral reasoning chal-
lenged cognitive-developmental stage theories of moral develop-
ment on two fronts. First, she argued for a broader conceptualiza-
tion of moral reasoning that encompassed a care orientation as well
as a justice orientation. The care orientation is characterized by a
focus on maintaining relationships, responding to the needs of
others, and a responsibility not to cause hurt. The justice orienta-
Sara Jaffee and Janet Shibley Hyde, Department of Psychology, Uni-
versity of Wisconsin—Madison.
We would like to acknowledge the contributions of Rose Jadack and
MaryBeth Nolan to this project. The Graduate School of the University of
Wisconsin provided financial support.
Correspondence concerning this article should be addressed to Janet
Shibley Hyde, Department of Psychology, University of Wisconsin—
Madison, 1202 W. Johnson Street, Madison, Wisconsin 53706.
tion is concerned with principles of fairness and equity such as
those assessed in conventional measures of moral reasoning (Gil-
ligan, 1982). Although acknowledging that males and females
could use either a justice or a care perspective, Gilligan asserted
that care reasoning was used predominantly by females and justice
reasoning was used predominantly by males (Gilligan, 1982; Gil-
ligan & Attanucci, 1988). Second, she argued that Kohlberg’s
Moral Judgment Interview (MJI; Colby et al., 1987)—the most
widely used measure of moral reasoning—was gender biased
because it was validated on an all-male sample and because its
scoring scheme characterized considerations of care and response
as less sophisticated than considerations of justice and fairness.
Over the last 15 years, Gilligan’s (1982) work has inspired a
wealth of empirical research and criticism, some of which has
supported her claims and some of which has not. At the same time,
researchers have seldom agreed on how Gilligan’s care and justice
orientations should be defined or how they should be measured.
Consequently, it has been difficult to compare the results of these
studies. The goal of this article is to evaluate Gilligan’s assertions
that (a) there are gender differences in Kohlbergian moral stage
and (b) the care and justice orientations are gender related. The
former assertion has been reviewed meta-analytically by several
investigators, and those results are summarized here. Meta-
analysis was used in the present paper to evaluate the latter
assertion. In addition, a number of complexities surrounding the
study of gender and moral reasoning are reviewed. The following
sections present Gilligan’s theory of moral reasoning, as well as
the criticisms of this work.
Gilligan’s Theory of Moral Reasoning
Gilligan’s (1977, 1982) theory of moral reasoning stemmed
from Kohlberg’s cognitive-developmental stage theory of moral
development. Kohlberg (1969, 1976, 1984) proposed that individ-
uals progress through a sequence of invariant and universal stages
of moral reasoning. These six stages are grouped into three levels,
each of which represents a qualitative advance in the individual’s
ability to understand and integrate diverse points of view (Kohl-
berg, 1976).
703
704 JAFFEE AND HYDE
Early research on Kohlbergian moral development reported that
Stage 3 was the modal stage for females and Stage 4 was the modal
stage for males (Fishkin, Keniston, & MacKinnon, 1973; Haan,
Smith, & Block, 1968; Holstein, 1969; Kohlberg & Kramer, 1969;
Poppen, 1974). Stage 3 reasoning is characterized by the desire to
maintain relationships and to meet others’ expectations, and
Stage 4 is characterized by a law-and-order mentality in which
laws are upheld so as to maintain the social order. These findings
led some researchers to accuse Kohlberg’s theory of gender bias
(Gilligan, 1977, 1982; Haan, 1978). Specifically, Gilligan (1977,
1982) argued that because Kohlberg derived his theory of moral
development from an all-male sample, he neglected to recognize a
distinctively female mode of moral reasoning—one that is char-
acterized by a desire to maintain relationships and a responsibility
not to cause hurt. This care orientation contrasts with a distinc-
tively male mode of moral reasoning—the justice orientation—
that is based on the abstract principles of justice, fairness, and
individualism captured by Kohlberg’s MJI. Unlike Kohlberg’s
moral stages, the care and justice orientations do not represent
cognitive structures that develop in a stagelike sequence. Instead,
they represent frameworks that can be modified by experience and
through which individuals interpret and resolve moral problems.
However, Gilligan argued that the care orientation’s emphasis on
maintaining relationships led to its classification as a Stage 3
response in Kohlberg’s scoring scheme.
Origins and Development of Moral Orientations
Gilligan and colleagues asserted that the care and justice orien-
tations are rooted in early childhood experiences of attachment and
inequality that foster, respectively, a relational and an individual-
istic self-concept (Gilligan & Attanucci, 1988; Gilligan & Wig-
gins, 1987). Because children are born into a position of inequality
and cannot survive without an attachment figure, all children are
exposed to the conditions that form the basis of both moral
orientations. Thus, all individuals have the capacity to understand,
experience, or implement either moral orientation. The reason that
males and females come to use one mode of moral reasoning over
the other is that these experiences of attachment and inequality are
differentially reinforced in a society dichotomized by gender.
Working from Chodorow’s (1978) neopsychoanalytic account of
gender identity development, Gilligan proposed that because
women are the primary caretakers in most cultures, girls’ self-
concepts are based on a definition of themselves as similar to and
connected with their mothers whereas boys’ self-concepts are
rooted in their difference and separation from their mothers. More-
over, boys identify with their fathers, who may be perceived as
authority figures who hold power over them. Thus, the experience
of attachment and connection is more salient to girls, whereas the
experience of inequality and separation is more salient to boys.
These initial experiences of attachment and inequality may be
confirmed in later childhood and adolescence, resulting in an
association between gender and moral orientation (Gilligan &
Wiggins, 1987). Gilligan and Wiggins (1987) concluded:
The sex difference question, when framed in this way, does not carry
the implication that one sex is morally superior, nor does it imply that
moral behavior is biologically determined. Instead, it draws attention
to two perspectives on morality. To the extent that biological sex, the
psychology of gender, and the cultural norms and values that define
masculine and feminine behavior affect the experience of equality and
attachment, these factors presumably will influence moral develop-
ment, (p. 282)
Empirical Tests of Gilligan’s Developmental Theory
These claims about the origins and development of moral voice
are largely untested. Benenson, Morash, and Petrakos (1998) ob-
served 41 mother-child dyads (children were 4 and 5 years old) in
a play setting and found that, compared with boys, girls were
physically closer to their mothers, engaged in more mutual eye
contact with their…